SOCIOMETRY AS SOCIAL-DEPTH
PSYCHOLOGY
Adam Blatner, M.D.
This is a supplement to a Workshop given for students of
the Drama Therapy Program at Antioch University California
Institute for Integral Studies, Seattle, Washington, October 29,
2012;
(It expands on comments for another workshop given
at California Institute for Integral Studies (CIIS) and
co-sponsored by the Living Arts Center in Oakland) at the CIIS
in San Francisco, on Sept. 24, 2012
Objectives:
(1) become more explicitly aware of how rapport influences
interpersonal and group interactions
(2) notice personal and cultural tendencies to avoid being aware
of these dynamics
a. fears of of embarrassment, vulnerability,
or potential feelings of rejection
b. concern for evoking the above feelings in
others
(3) develop insight into some of the underlying reasons for the
various reactions to becoming sensitive to this level of
social-depth psychology
(4) consider how to help clients and colleagues apply awareness
of these dynamics in therapy groups and other contexts
* For a more thorough treatment of this subject, please read my
writings on sociometry on my website:
Introduction
Sociometry is a method for assessing the nature of
interpersonal and inter-group relationships, based on asking
about and plotting on a chart the invisible patterns of
preferences in a group. This method was developed in the
mid-1930s by Jacob L. Moreno, M.D., who also who also invented
psychodrama, was a pioneer in writing about role theory, group
psychotherapy, and the theory of creativity and spontaneity.
Sociometry became a recognized method in sociology through the
mid-1960s although its use has become relatively obscure since
that time.
(More papers on sociometry may be found elsewhere
on this website.)
Sociometry is less-well-known because it refers to phenomena
that are between what is addressed by most types of psychology
of the individual person and the field addressed by sociology.
Second, sociometry refers to two levels of meaning: It is a
method for assessing the nature and relative strengths of
attractions and repulsions in relationships and groups (i.e.,
Moreno called these “tele” and I use the term “rapport”). In a
broader sense, though, the word refers to the vast field of
psycho-social dynamics that partake of the underlying theme of
preference.
To illustrate the nature of the two meanings of sociometry, I
draw your attention to an analogy. In the mid-19th century the
microscope as a technology was improved so that bacteria could
be seen—and this was a revelation. It opened our eyes to what
had been previously invisible. But as folks learned about germs,
they found that there was much about the field of microbiology
that could not be illuminated by even the most powerful
microscopes. Viruses, for example, were part of this field, and
the chemistry and ecology and other aspects again involved
qualities that were not observable to any type of microscope.
So, too, there are many aspects of social-depth psychology—my
term for the far broader field—that cannot be illuminated by
sociometric methods as Moreno developed them.
Nevertheless, Moreno is credited for addressing this fairly
obvious phenomenon: When you go into a large group, you “click”
with some people, feel repelled by others, and most are
somewhere in-between. What’s all that about? Rapport, of course,
but what is really going on here? That’s what we’re beginning to
explore.
The first point to emphasize, then, is simply that mainstream
psychology has hardly addressed this realm of interpersonal
dynamics. Eric Berne’s semi-social “Transactional Analysis” only
touched on this realm, but mainly in terms of the interface of
individual needs and an unfortunate application of the idea of
“games people play” to these dynamics. It’s good, but it’s only
one small aspect of this vast field. From mid-20th century
psychoanalysis, Harry Stack Sullivan emphasized the
“inter-personal” nature of much psychology, but again it tended
to overly focus on how an individual may distort what’s going
on. These approaches lacked the capacity to notice more elusive
and often realistic currents of interpersonal dynamics. Of
course, in another sense, there’s nothing subtle about them—they
have to do with first impressions, love at first sight, taking
an immediate dislike to someone, and so forth. It’s just that we
don’t know how to talk about such interactions.
Overlaps With Other Fields
Fields of study overlap, so that, say, chemistry, overlaps
with hundreds of other fields. Social- depth psychology also
overlaps with many other related fields, such as:
- developmental psychology, especially in the social
arena, the arena of play, of how kids choose playmates, friends,
and the impact of being chosen or rejected has on the
personality
- anthropology, the development of criteria for status,
rank, power, oppression, in various institutions in various
cultures
- communications, language shifts for speaking to those
with more or less status or rank
- gender studies, women’s studies, and studies of other
oppressed groups
- studies of temperament and its impact socially,
introversion, extroversion, different cognitive or personality
styles
- management theory and ways to promote creativity and
group morale based on actual psychology rather than mechanistic
yet obsolete views of “efficiency”
- educational theory, like management theory
- psychotherapy (a very broad field of people-helping),
and coaching
- various schools of psychology and psychotherapy,
sub-schools of psychoanalysis, especially “object relations”
theory and “self psychology.”
- sociology and social psychology (Many
factors)
- cultural history, how many of what has been related to
the phenomena on this list is changing as roles shift, status
shifts, generations evolve, technology adds new gimmicks and has
increasing impact especially with younger people
Problems With Social-Depth Psychology
These interactions are emotionally very sensitive! Consider
how, a century ago as Freud was introducing the idea that sexual
impulses, there was a lot of resistance; the topic was somewhat
taboo. Nevertheless, Freudians correctly (on this) noted that it
was no good pretending that sexuality didn’t exist. Denial of
this dynamic could and did lead to neurosis. People needed to
know that children did have sexual feelings, and also that
people often entertained sexual feelings that needed to be kept
secret for fear of general disapproval. While this cultural
avoidance has weakened a bit, it’s still there; and some might
say that attention given to sexuality has even gone to the other
extreme. Apart from sex, or other tender questions—politics and
religion, most famously, we should note the phenomenon of
avoidance, of taboo.
Who prefers whom for what reasons is similarly avoided lest
people get their feelings hurt. Yet pretending that these
dynamics don’t exist is as pervasive and unrealistic and
productive of various forms of social and individual disturbance
as the repression of sexuality. Consider the degrees of
emotional tenderness associated with some of the questions noted
at the end in Appendix B.
We should not underestimate the depth, the unconscious
activity, and the emotional tenderness of social-depth
psychological phenomena. First, the mind is an exquisitely
sensitive receptor of micro-non-verbal communications—and maybe
even pheromones. The mind is a social organ. I suspect that
nearly everyone has unresolved residues of not being chosen, not
“clicking” with relatives or others that one expected to be able
to feel more positive rapport, of feeling more isolated at times
than could be explained, and the great vulnerability associated
with these kinds of experiences.
Freud made enemies by calling attention to what everyone
knew and it just wasn’t nice to talk about. We don’t go
there; we don’t even think about such things. Of course,
people did “go there” in their minds and there was little
opportunity to think about them without feeling guilty. Some
managed to keep it under wraps but didn’t let it get to
them—they were just up-tight, and like some hypocrites
today, outwardly extra fussy about any breaking of the
rules. Others couldn’t handle it and were consumed with
guilt or shame, and this came out in all manner of neuroses.
Freud was a bit right, but he was wrong in coming to a
premature conclusion that sex or aggression were the only
issues. Adler tried to point out the desire for being
one-up, power politics. (His psychology is almost relevant
to what we’re talking about.) Jung noted that spiritual
experiences and temperament were also important. Freud and
his colleagues interpreted these rather valid additions as
mere rebellions against the father. Later, many revolutions
within the psychoanalytic movements also broadened the
issues being addressed: There was a recognition of the need
to have a close relations (i.e., “object relations theory,”
which is close in another way to what we’ll be talking
about); a recognition that people need to feel themselves as
valued and coherent (i.e., “self psychology”), and so forth.
More recently people are recognizing that they’re all
correct—there are many “basic” instincts. My point is that
there are also basic psycho-dynamics that operate in the
interpersonal and group field, dynamics that have to do with
interpersonal preferences.
But, like sex, or vulnerability, or an allergy to shame,
people are reluctant to think or talk about such matters.
The idea that we are not preferred by others is an affront
to our pride, even though it’s clear in another part of our
mind that we in turn do not prefer most people very much,
and we distinctly do not like some. But if anyone feels that
way to us, we are hurt, and this hurt can be deep and
repressed.
I think Moreno over-rode this whole dimension and failed to
appreciate how very tender these issues are for most people.
He was pathologically both narcissistic and a bit dense
interpersonally, so he was not deeply hurt by the many
people who found his over-enthusiasm and inclination to
over-value his own gifts to be somewhat obnoxious. On the
good side, these qualities gave him the resilience that most
people lack, so he kept going—to our lasting benefit. Thus
sometimes people’s weaknesses end up serving them as
strengths. Life’s funny that way.
But the point to be made is that Moreno sort of acted as if
all we had to do was to be open about our preferences and we
can all work this out. He didn’t really see the pervasive
emotional sensitivity and many layers of avoidance that were
built into social depth-psychology. All I’m doing is
acknowledging that (1) he was right: there’s a lot there and
if we could be open about it, a lot would change; and (2) he
greatly overestimated the depth, pervasiveness, and
sensitivity of the dynamics involved. So what I’m doing in
this talk is to give more attention to the great complexity
of the field and the extent of the problems involved.
Trying to explain these in terms of psychoanalytic
theory is in my mind quite inadequate, because that theory
doesn’t sufficiently deal with the reality that most people
don’t particularly enjoy most other people! Some people do enjoy
some people, and sorting out who goes most comfortably with whom
is a challenge that has never been addressed systematically.
Again, there’s the analogy to microbiology: When folks didn’t
know about germs, they just suffered through all types of
infections. Systems of sewage management and cleanliness emerged
based on germ theory. So, too, our present culture doesn’t yet
appreciate any systematic way to address ways of fostering
rapport and allowing people who feel more congenial with each
other to find each other. Now there are some “speed dating”
services and other articles and approaches that touch on the
problem, but for the most part schools and businesses don’t much
deal with the individual needs of their students or workers, but
rather serve the convenience of the faculty or managers.
As creativity becomes more prized, though, several things become
clear—things pointed out by Moreno in the 1930s through the
1950s: People work better together when they have good “tele”
(Moreno’s term for positive rapport); and conversely, when there
is a mismatch, friction, the work is far less creative or even
stymied. Group morale increases when people can find more
commonalities, ways of connecting. Teamwork also improves when
folks can choose more freely those with whom they will
affiliate.
Still, there is little consensus about the dynamics involved—few
know about “sociometry” or recognize Moreno’s name. There is no
general vocabulary for the field. And this field is situated too
much in the realm of individual depth psychology for most social
psychologists, and too social for most individual-psychology
oriented researchers. The truth is that these dynamics do
involve both fields.
Role Theory
Moreno also was a pioneer of role theory, which offers a
relatively user-friendly language for addressing the problems of
understanding social-depth psychology. Role theory also suffers
from the problem of being somewhat elusive because it addresses
phenomena at and between and among many different levels of
body-mind-family-subculture-organization-culture-species
organization. We do play roles that partake of many levels—human
mental and social behavior is not compartmentalized. For
example, whereas loyalties and issues relating to smaller
relevant groups tend to dominate, under major stress, whole
demographic categories, women, the nation, a religion, whatever,
will claim not only the loyalty, but major acts in the service
of that loyalty, sometimes to the detriment of one’s local
social network. Families in the U.S. Civil War in the mid-19th
century sometimes were split regarding who fought for or
supported the North or the South.
I've systematically developed as “role dynamics,” and
though I mainly draw from Moreno’s ideas, I also take into
consideration the work of others, such as Theodore Sarbin and
many others. I think role theory stopped being used in sociology
because it was too elusive, it addressed phenomena at too many
other levels. This fault in academia is a virtue in a practical
sense because role theory addresses phenomena operating at and
between and among all levels of human psycho-socio-cultural
organization. There are roles in the body-mind, roles within the
person, roles between persons, in small groups, larger groups,
etc. Each has its own properties but also overlaps with other
roles. In fact, no clear distinctions exist between most
categories that have to do with mind, which makes psycho-social
research difficult. Still, that’s just the nature of the
subject. Mind does interpenetrate much more actively on multiple
levels. (One might discern all other things interpenetrating
also, but it’s not so flagrant.) .
Other “Resistances” to Social-Depth Psychology
First is the general discomfort with any new idea or field.
People don’t like thinking about that which they are not
prepared to think about, and there are a number of component
concepts here that one needs to learn to get used to.
There’s also the tendency to slip into stupidity, which is the
illusion that what one knows is sufficient. Many academics have
failed to balance their erudition with humility and lapse into
thinking that the body of knowledge accumulated in their field
and others they know about constitutes pretty much all that
needs to be known. The gut feeling is “what do I need to know
about that nonsense for?” Lots of otherwise quite intelligent
people in the mid-19th century resisted the theory of germs.
They were invisible, for goodness sakes! It took decades of
patient and persistent work to get professionals—alas, many the
ancestors of my profession of medicine!—to get on board. The
same will have to happen to social-depth psychology. Add to this
the deep discomfort at having to recognized the gut-level
operation of non-rational feelings, attractions that one
shouldn’t have, repulsions that one often doesn’t want to admit
even to oneself!
An equally problematic issue is the residual and still-common
belief that we are primarily conscious and are and should be
able to control our preferences as well as our overt behavior.
There is a hubris about the mind, a product of the ideals of
modernity and a residue of the ethos of self-control that
emerged in the late 19th century. The idea that we are in great
part unable to choose how to feel is still confused with the
vague sense of not being held responsible. (We may ask ourselves
to be civil and courteous regardless of feelings—a skill
cultivated by mature adults that is ever-more needed as our
world becomes multi-cultural. But that doesn’t mean we can make
ourselves prefer or like those with whom we feel little or even
negative rapport.) Keeping these problems in mind, we can then
proceed to wondering about what goes on with people.
Preferences Beyond the Interpersonal
Field
First, we need to know that it isn’t just people who have
preferences. Even one-celled animals exhibit preference in what
they eat and whom they mate with. Also, humans have preferences
for all sorts of things besides people—colors, foods, hair
styles, etc. Some of these are cultural fashions, but even
within cultures there are variations in preference. Temperament
also may be viewed in part as a variable in preference for
noise, other people, excitements, etc.The whole
thing about preferences is that they exist with other people,
but in a much broader way preferences operate for thousands of
other categories—foods, music, decor, hair style, etc.
Impersonal types aren’t reciprocated. They exist also for
groups, but here it gets complex. Rarely does a person agree
with all the things that the group wants to do or support. So
there’s a complexity in that there are selected elements that
have positive preference and often some in which there is
repulsion. It’s all on a continuum, also. Many things are
supported that are not really cared about one way or the other.
Even in relationships, there are numerous preferences that are
enhanced because they are shared, and some that operate
independently—one person cares but not the other, but it is of
no matter; and sometimes different or opposing preferences
generate conflict. So it’s important to broaden Moreno’s concept
of “tele” a bit.
Becoming Aware of Your Own Preferences
We should not assume that people are clear about their
preferences. Most people change preferences about some things
over time, and sometimes it takes months or years for them to
become aware of this shift. Sometimes it’s just that what was
preferred isn’t so valued now that one has “had enough.” There
is the “been there, done that” shift. The point here is that the
activity of helping people to connect with what they actually
prefer now is an interesting challenge. It can be a vocational
challenge, or a matter for spiritual guidance.
Part of the problem for many people is that what they actually
do prefer is sometimes at variance with what they think they
should prefer, or what others prefer. Individuation has a price:
Sometimes valued people will disapprove or be uncomfortable with
your changes in preferences.
The problem of preference expands and becomes more complex
when we join two or more preference-exercising minds together.
Then we get occasional flashes of “Oh, you like this too?” and
often non-rational flashes of “I find you attractive”—and these
attractions often are not sexual or romantic—they can be—but
based on other qualities. When two people reciprocate in the
attraction, that increases the dynamic lure of the invisible
connection.
We all sense this at a big get-together. Some folks we find we
have more natural rapport with, and others who have done nothing
consciously off-putting nevertheless put us off—we have a mild
to severe repugnance or negative rapport. Most fall into the
category of indifferent—they’re okay I guess but I’m inclined to
do little to try to get to know them.
These generalities, though, are riddled with all sorts of
counter patterns:
- those who try to play hostess and include those who
seem lost or unconnected
- those who try to make friends with those with whom they
feel frightened or put off, a kind of “reaction formation”
through which they try to cope with their fear
- those who play up to those who seem most popular, even
if they don’t feel any actual attraction or sense of
reciprocated interest—Moreno called this
“aristo-tele.”
Other Dynamics Relating to Social-Depth Psychology
The key themes are obvious: We want to be liked. When we
are not liked in the way we want to be liked, we are hurt. On
the other hand, there are many people whom we don’t particularly
like—and most of them we don’t dislike, either. But some we do
dislike. Nor can we adequately explain why we dislike someone.
This overlaps with another bunch of dynamics. People bother us
in a wide variety of ways, and it may seem that they know it, or
they’re doing it on purpose. But really, most of the time they
don’t know it, they don’t mean to bother us, and often we cannot
even say what they’re doing that bothers us. These issues run
very deep.
This overlaps with another bunch of dynamics: We rationalize, we
come up with reasons. It happens so fast we don’t even know
we’re doing it. We come up with reasons that bolster our
attractions. We write love poetry or make up political speeches
for our favorite candidates. And we come up with all sorts of
reasons that justify why we don’t like someone. More primitive
people come up with rather crude ideas, stereotypes, obvious
prejudices, the almost evolutionary-based instincts of
xenophobia, disliking those who are not like whoever we think of
as “us.”
Boys age six may go through a phase where they dislike girls,
who are stinky. This is not a matter of smell, this is an
unconscious need to identify us and how we are better than them.
It’s built in.
Let’s just say right now that a great deal of what holds up
civilization also unconsciously functions to temper these
childish and primitive tendencies. We develop ethics and beyond
that, elaborate systems of courtesy. We don’t want to be hurt,
so we generate elaborate practices to avoid hurting or
humiliating others. In some more status-stratified cultures, the
more worthy treat the less-worth with meticulous care, and in
return, the less-worthy show ritualized forms of honor. But
there are scores of currents for dealing with strangers,
language, hospitality, indirectness of language, and so forth.
Courtesy has probably saved far more lives than all the armies
ever marshaled against enemies.
Considering Social-Depth Psychology
First, now that I make it explicit, it’s clear to everyone
immediately that who likes whom and why involves very tender
issues. Connect this with another obvious point: Awareness
related to these dynamics are both individually and collectively
repressed. We wouldn’t want to hurt anyone’s feelings, now,
would we?
In spite of that tendency to avoid conflict, we need to directly
challenge that complex, because if it’s not brought into
explicit consciousness, talked about, considered and
re-considered, all sorts of collective sociopathology happens.
Reactions to mistaken interpretations of behaviors associated
with interpersonal preference become poisonous and come out in
weird and wacky ways. Also, I want to submit that dynamics
having to do with social-depth psychology get displaced into a
significant percentage of what is recognized as social or
individual psychopathology, and which is not explained
adequately by other theories.
People want to be esteemed, respected, liked, preferred, and if
this does not happen, they tend to feel hurt, diminished,
resentful, confused, lonely, and so forth. Let’s note from the
get-go that most people by age 5 do not in turn like everyone
else in their social network in a strongly positive,
unqualified, loving manner. Even little babies have preferences
as to who they feel safe with, who they like to handle them—and
who makes them feel creepy and want to cry. We all have
preferences.
Developmental Dynamics
The second point is that we have a funny double standard.
It’s okay for us to have preferences, but if others prefer
others over us—especially how we were when we were little—we
feel hurt, jealous, outraged, diminished—it’s a narcissistic
wound. Two points here. One, this is normal, okay, part of a
necessary step in development—at some point we’re egocentric. We
may also be well socially integrated, but that doesn’t counter
the immaturity of our interpretation. We can’t help it, we were
little. The second point: Lots of people never get fully over
this, in part because it’s rarely talked about. It’s a bit like
sexuality in this way. Most people sort of get past being
dominated by childish reaction patterns, at least outwardly, but
do they understand deeply that Johnny might prefer to play with
Sally instead of me for certain things? No! I’m hurt! Crushed!
Humiliated! Jealous! And I can’t admit any of this to myself
much less anyone else.
In some people this whole dynamic is much stronger than in
others, and it depends on temperament and compensatory roles and
many other circumstances. Some contain these feelings, some
become pretty neurotic or express it through a personality
disorder that makes others suffer. But everyone has at least a
trace of this dynamic—it’s a stage we all have gone through. The
main point is that it’s good to know about it so that we can
navigate this transition with a bit more maturity. Like grief
work, it cannot be done purely rationally. It needs to be lived
through. What is involved is a shift from early childhood
egocentricity to more mature role differentiation: We like some
people more than others for certain things, and others feel the
same way towards us.
The "Oedipal Complex"
Freud noted that kids around age 5 tend to want to have sex with
their mothers and are afraid of their fathers cutting their
penises off and that this was universal with boys. With girls,
they have an Electra complex and want to have sex with their
fathers and fear their mothers already cut off their penis.
Later psychoanalysts like Karen Horney rejected this. But Freud
was right about something---though his interpretation was wrong.
Around that age kids move from parallel play with playmates to
playing with small groups and jealousy rears its head again if
it didn't too much with the birth of siblings. The
aforementioned disconnect applies. We want all our friends to
prefer us but in fact we prefer our friends differently, A
prefers B to play this way, but C to play that way; and
occasionally, as will happen, he notices that B prefers C in
ways that make him feel left out! This kind of triangulation is
inevitable and for a time generates confusion and hurt. Freud
was right about this happening pretty universally, but it wasn't
about the parents, it was about peers. And the genitals are not
particularly important.
Later on boys and girls do get into gender differentiation, so
that to boys girls are icky, and vice versa, but this
generalization really is only slightly true. Many non-homosexual
boys like some girls for some kinds of non-sexual non-romantic
play and vice versa, and this cross-gender equality or
preference is not complete---but it may be common. There are
some guys who like women as pals, and some women who prefer to
be with guys rather than other women. It's not usually
pronounced, but it's discernable. Then there are guys who like
to pal with guys and gals with gals except for romance or sex.
So generalizations are best not made other than to note that
these variations and others are common.
Now that’s the general situation. It has innumerable
permutations and expressions. The appendix has a questionnaire
that might warm us all up to the problem.
This is surprisingly heavy material. The attachments, losses,
breakups—either those we initiated (along with degrees of anger
or guilt) and those that were initiated by the other against our
will— all leave significant residues in our minds and hearts,
sometimes more vividly than relationships with parents!
Some Other Themes
Social-depth psychology touches on very significant
problems in social psychology, such as the following:
:
- respect, what we do to get it, how much of
what kind do we want
- liking, including the feelings associated with being
liked or desired for things that you don’t want to be noticed or
made a fuss about!
- status, and what you feel that brings you or entitles
you to
- privilege, and what comes easy to you, or which
privileges are not easily gained or granted by society regarding
age, race, gender, sexual orientation, profession, class, parts
of the country you come from, etc.
- rank in an organization, what ranks you have attained
in your life, ranks you failed to attain, including offices in
an organization, roles, winning of contests, diplomas,
certificates, awards
- other forms of public recognition, or feelings relating
to not having been adequately recognized
- counter-roles, anti-authoritarian, rebels, the dropouts
who smoked cigarettes behind the
cafeteria (Did you have peers who
echoed your sentiments?)
- fame, to local or national degree, or beyond
- size of your social media, acquaintance volume, how
many people know you by name, by face
- anything that you are envied for, height, figure, sports
prowess, wealth, savoir-faire, dancing, etc. Anything that
you are pitied for. What you think is so, versus what anyone
else might have said.
-
Some of these may seem irrelevant to you, trivial even. Some may
hit a nerve.
Further Comments---"Take Away Principles"
A Deep Disconnect
1. Many people have a disconnect between childhood
attitudes about being liked and the growing vague awareness that
one prefers only some people and doesn’t prefer many others, not
because of any distinct fault in the others but simply because
one’s own interests involve a smaller fraction of the general
populace. This ratio of how many people seem relevant out of the
number of people one is aware of becomes rapidly smaller; that
is, as mid-childhood proceeds, the acquaintance volume increases
rapidly and those who seem relevant makes up an ever-smaller
fraction.
But the early pattern of mild egocentricity and narcissistic
sensitivity continues unless it’s noticed. It’s stronger in
intensity for people whose childhoods are afflicted with parents
who are themselves narcissistic, or for those who have
experienced a lot of shame, or for other reasons. (These
stronger propensities are addressed by the literature of
psychoanalysis, especially associated with the Kohutian
“self-psychology” school of thought.) My point is that it’s not
particularly pathological in most people, but its retention does
generate a degree of unrealistic social self-consciousness and
sensitivity. Not only is a degree of pride, self-esteem, and
status factors to be noted more consciously, but young people
need to get past this egocentric stage of hypersensitivity to
what is perceived to be less delight from everyone.
The disconnect, to say again, is that while we all want
unqualified preference, enjoyment, or liking from everyone, at
around the same time we ourselves find that we prefer only
certain people and not others. The connection involves more
deeply recognizing that others do this too. So, there’s no shame
or implied blame of any type involved with not being the high
preference of others. The retention of egocentricity and
narcissistic tendencies makes human relations a bit prickly.
We should recognize that people’s subconscious awareness of how
poignant and consequential hurting someone else’s pride, making
them “lose face,” or in other ways communicating disrespect or a
lack of what is expected in the way of expressions of respect
accounts for a considerable fraction of what is known as tact,
courtesy, etiquette, good manners, political savvy, and so
forth. Becoming more explicitly conscious rather than dimly
conscious allows us to achieve several things:
- We may become less sensitive to slights to our own
pride system. This is important for our own mental health, and
also more relevant in a more multi-cultural society where signs
of respect in one culture may not be known to those in another
culture.
- It becomes useful from a psychology perspective to
appreciate how the mind is an exquisitely sensitive social
organ. Mental-emotional processes can be better managed in
proportion to people knowing the realities of mind and social
relationships.
- To say again more explicitly: We prefer those who share
values and interests and for a few other reasons, such as
kinship. We don’t condemn, but neither do we prefer most other
people— they simply are not “our kind.” It is a phony
expectation (born of pseudo-egalitarianism?) to want to be liked
by everyone. We only like a few, even though we may be generally
well-disposed to most, and wanting some expression of kindness
or civil courtesy is okay; wanting to be liked or preferred by
most is unrealistic. (This may be distorted by the emergence of
mass media and along with it, the prevalence of celebrities and
the illusion that one can be liked by nearly everyone.)
The main value of social-depth psychology and the field exposed
by sociometry is that people can be helped to repair the
disconnect and modulate easier those childish desires to be
preferred and admired by everyone. From this one can begin to
notice one’s own social sensitivity and to consciously modify
the residual emotion.
These points are not all tightly connected,
please note.
The Value of Appreciating Social-Depth Psychology
The more I think about this field, the more I realize that
the society remains in a high degree of a mixture of ignorance,
repression and denial about fairly common realities. I noted
that feelings of being not valued, liked, liked enough,
delighted in, respected, admired, are ubiquitious. They hurt
enough so as to be repressed and denied. Most of these are
sensed subconsciously and remain over-generalized. Only through
conscious review can we demote what was intuitively taken as a
slap in the face to the status of simply not being noticed or
flattered sufficiently. People do take things personally and
they don’t even know it; nor can they avoid feeling hurt and
closing down or experiencing resentment simply by being
admonished, “Don’t take it personally,” or “Don’t be so
sensitive.” These reactions are often unconscious and so the
conscious reaction is just to get tight, un-feeling, armored,
and to not let others know that it bothers you. Indeed, you tend
not to even know consciously that it does bother you. It’s okay.
The problem is that what you do consciously is feeble when it
comes to this deep sensitivity.
So the challenge begins by bringing the whole problem into the
common sphere of discourse. This took fifty or more years
regarding sexuality. At least it is getting talked about. Yes,
at times it seems to be a matter of going overboard, of almost
over-emphasizing sexuality, of recognizing sex “addiction,” etc.
But it happened way back then, too, it just wasn’t so obvious.
Freud helped us liberate the theme of sexuality, but he didn’t
touch on the theme of pride. Adler got closer to this theme, but
it deserves far more elaboration in terms of wide understanding.
It’s not just a reaction to feelings of inferiority; sensitivity
to status and being liked and admired is build into the mind of
the higher animals as well as humans! (E.g., pecking order,
systems of dominance and submission, etc.) So let’s begin by
admitting it and addressing it more consciously.
Overlapping Fields
Sociology, Social Psychology, Depth Psychology—one thing
Moreno may be give credit for is that he helped break down the
artificial barriers among these fields. Sociometry bridges these
and other activities and phenomena. What it doesn’t directly
address, it then leaves open as questions: What are the
phenomena associated with fashionability, status, prejudice,
familiarity, the power of propinquity (proximity in space)? Now
that the internet has made relationships across the miles
possible, what are the equivalents of propinquity today?
The culture of celebrity has heightened the phenomena associated
with what Moreno called “aristo-tele,” wanting to be associated
with someone who is more popular or has higher status. If one
cannot boast of “knowing first-hand” some popular person, one
can still pick up depth-psychology “points” just by being a
“fan.” We should not underestimate the power of this second- and
third-hand illusion of affiliation. People’s pride at “their”
sports team—not as owners, but simply as fans—constitute a great
deal of the psychic energy of humanity.
At this point, I am inclined to remain quite open to discoveries
about status, being liked, liking, interpersonal preferences—the
elements that make up social depth psychology or sociometry—as
affected by many fields:
- anthropology
- in a culture characterized by inter-cultural
mobility, inter-class, inter-sub-culture mobility, and the
fashionability of certain perceived marginalized sub-cultures
(“goth,” “druggie” “hoodie” etc.), many people adopt values
antithetical to their parents or their parents’ friends.
- urban, suburban, rural, sophisticated,
provincial, etc.
- degrees of education, erudition, and types of
contempt for degrees, professions, education, etc.
- relationship to psychology and social
“with-it-ness,” or a contempt for such values
- relationship to religion, other religions,
spirituality apart from organized religion, mysticism, and
supernatural “illusions,” etc.
- semantics, the meanings of words as used by
different sub-cultures, etc. (For example, mysticism is taken as
pejorative by many, and associated to different meanings, as
complimentary by some.)
- what counts as status consciously and
unconsciously even in subcultures that seem to have moved beyond
obvious status markers
- shifts in fashions in clothes, jewelry, body-piercings,
tattoos, hair styles, etc.
- the changing status in various subcultures of smoking,
drinking, being drunk, etc.
. . . etc.
These are indeed related to sociometry insofar as we have strong
conscious and often unconscious preferences for people aside
from common interests. I will confess that given a good lead
with common interests, I have a desire not to be in personal
contact with someone who smokes, or who reeks of smoke. I can
respect that person in some ways, but would prefer not to be in
contexts that require direct contact. A committee mainly
mediated by electronic technology? Okay. That sort of thing,
with many variations, goes on all the time in our changing
culture.
I Enjoy You Like That
It is so special to find another person who can say that.
Some folks have not had this pleasure, alas. And it can be so
very different for different folks. Do others enjoy you for what
you’d like to be enjoyed for?
- your preferences and style of travel
- going fishing and being together quietly
- going to movies, the same kind
- preparing food together, or eating
- getting drunk together
- your way of enjoying sexuality
- your style of being affectionate or not
- your choice in reading materials, books
- your degree of adventure or lack thereof
- your type of physical challenges or thrills
- your choice of television shows
- your choice of magazines
- enjoying family together
- enjoying extended family visits
- avoiding the same things
- exchanging and giving presents
... etc.
Probably I missed several categories. The
aforementioned list is meant to have been suggestive rather than
definitive, to encourage you to come up with items that fit your
situation.
This varies greatly, and is a clue to “compatibility,” which
involves the presence of more than a significant
percentage of pleasantly symmetrical (we like doing together) or
complementary (he does this while I do that and together we’re a
team) role connections. There is always some non-symmetry and
non-complementarity, and the question is whether the number and
significance of the roles where there isn’t compatibility is
that important. There is no external judge of what counts and
how much—each couple works it out for themselves. This applies
to friendships, siblings, not just marriages.
Another point here is to confront our tendency to assume that
others do or at least should like what we value and dislike what
we disdain; but they don’t. And being curious and open to the
deeper and more mind-stretching meaning of different strokes for
different folks is broadening. You mean there are people who
actually like that? And others who don’t like this? Wow!
There is a potential and experience that most psychologists
don’t recognize—the experience of spontaneity. Mihaly
Csikszentmihaly calls a certain sector of this spectrum “flow,”
but I would add that the full field of spontaneity involves many
other elements:
- making a spontaneous joke and finding that others
find it funny
- making a funny face or humming a song and finding
you have delighted your friend or your beloved—and you weren’t
even trying
- coming up with something to say or do and it was
the very right response, and you consciously didn’t know what to
say
- getting into the groove and dancing better than
you consciously knew how, or singing along with more vigor or
flavor than you knew you could, or doodling something that came
out better than you thought you could, and so forth.
- and so forth.
These are important experiences and they can be explained, but
we need to stretch beyond our generally recognized limits of
knowledge. There are potentials in the mind that may have to do
with the astrocyte glial cells of the brain, or the fifth chakra
of the subtle energy system, or the extra-corporeal inspirations
of the muses, or partial possession by ancestral spirits—so much
has to do with the belief systems of your culture. (I don’t
suppose the more physicalistic Western scientific culture to be
necessarily more “right” on this point.) The point is that
spontaneity, inspiration, and creativity happens, and much in
the modern realms of business and industry are actively seeking
ways to support the emergence of more of it.
Moreno in a way foresaw this, or at least noted its possibility,
and also the modern industrial and politico-religious culture
that suppresses spontaneity and creativity. You can’t open to
these inflows of inspiration if you’re at all scared or
defensive. The mid-modern culture still used fear—the fear of
losing, of missing out, of being punished ,or excluded from
advancement, etc., as well as the fear of being teased,
humiliated, exposed, etc.—as a major motivation for effort in
the world. Future generations will undoubtedly will view getting
beyond fear-based motivation as the next step of individual and
collective liberation, following the liberation from slavery of
any kind, discrimination, prejudice, etc. Dare we anticipate
this? One way to help humanity toward that goal is to name it:
Let’s get beyond fear-based motivation!
But what else is there? How about the fun of creativity? How
about a safer culture free of coercion? Is this close to John
Lennon’s song, Imagine? The concept of spontaneity is subversive
of fear-based systems in general, because it can’t flower within
contexts that don’t feel safe. In turn, in a culture that values
and needs innovation, the foundation again involves not talking
up how great creativity is, but rather working to create the
circumstances that will support people’s natural inclination to
play, to explore, to invent, to make up stuff—this is the
natural heritage. It cannot be forced!
Social Preference
Another factor that may be understood in this light is the
way folks sort themselves out according to preferences if given
half the chance. This was the point of Moreno’s sociometry! Many
social institutions interfered with this natural process, using
the rationalization of fairness to impose what in reality were
rules that served the convenience of the administrators. Still
today teachers, managers, and group leaders assign teams rather
than let them pick each other intuitively.
This is more complex, of course, because some people remain
under-chosen and what, then, to do with them? Interestingly,
often given a chance to sort themselves out according to modes
of play, they will often choose each other. Some will need to
come to terms with whether they actually fit in with certain
groups, and perhaps the option of finding groups outside the
main group need to be developed. Another option is to open the
criteria and do a bit of exploration: Some of the under-chosen
or even rejected turn out to have useful skills that they
haven’t yet learned to manifest or disclose.
Dr. Shelly Korshack and Marianne Shapiro recently wrote about
the under-chosen and how else that might be managed. It started
me thinking about this problem. Their point is good: It's
worthwhile exploring the possibility that some who are
under-chosen might well have something interesting and positive
to offer. However, since this problem happens, here are some
thoughts:
The under-chosen involve many sub-groups, and
what they are doing is good—allowing a group to consider the
criteria and elements of preference. But let’s just be deeply
provocative.
Person A has real odor issues due to great
neglect of personal mouth or body hygiene. How does this get
dealt with?
Person B has developed intimidating nonverbal
habits, a screeching voice, glowering look, a sneer.
Person C has a mixture of elements, plus not
wanting much to socialize—finding fault with just about all of
“them.” Mutually rejecting, isolate.
Person D is well liked in another circle, but
feels no need to hide that s/he has deeply anomalous political
or religious or other deep value views from the main group.
Etc.
Further Horizons
After pondering Moreno’s ideas for several decades, I think
he’s opened some doors but yet has not fully treated all that
operates in these fields—nor should he be expected to. It might
have been better if he had directly and repeatedly admitted that
what he’s done is a beginning, not a final cap on this analysis.
Sociometry has two meanings: In the narrower sense, it refers to
the method, and more particularly, a method that assesses
interpersonal preferences based on a given criterion within a
group. In the larger sense, sociometry refers to the larger
field of social psychology, especially that arena characterized
by preferences.
I think it would be better to recognize, though, that the larger
field involves many phenomena that are in no way elucidated by
any sociometric methods developed either by Moreno or many of
his followers. This is okay: It doesn’t really diminish the
contributions so far, but rather opens the door to considering
all sorts of phenomena beyond the narrower scope of Moreno’s
sociometry.
It is sort of social psychology, but it also bridges into the
realm of depth psychology. Perhaps this is one reason why
theories of individual psychology and theories of social
psychology have missed this—the phenomena is truly bridging
between both fields: How do you feel about not being chosen by
those you wish would choose you? How do you feel about not
really preferring those whom you sense would like you to prefer
them? These are important dynamics that have to do with
hurt feelings, resentment, courtesy (so that others won’t be
hurt or dis-respected), often unconscious sadism,
jealousy, envy, and all sorts of combinations of these and other
feelings.
Action explorations on the surface seems to involve techniques
for problem-solving, but deeper dynamics involve processes that
involve developing group cohesion, a sense of safety, support,
and being liked and enjoyed. Positive social feelings support
the emergence of creativity. Applause and rooting sections do
evoke more courage, spontaneity and showmanship! So in this
sense, the creative energy in action explorations depends upon
the cultivation of a foundation of social integration—which
brings us back to social depth-psychology.
Adam Smith noted that in the mainstream, ordinary bargaining was
a pro-social force, the give and take of mainstream civilized
society. No competition, but non-sentimental collaboration. Add
to this the force of empathy, sentimental collaboration. I enjoy
your happiness. We feel this more with those we care about, but
there is a generous and kindly spirit in many communities where
the population is not overly worried or insecure. So take this
two steps further into the positive: (1) the act of
collaborating in the pursuit of a common goal, and (2) adding
more or less improvisation and creative problem-solving toward
that goal.
So this chapter focuses on the foundation, the social-depth
psychology that supports the method. A single technique does not
operate in a vacuum. One needs a number of component elements to
be in place and maintained, and a true artist integrates. The
less mature hope for and at times are led to expect short-cuts,
a single technique that fits all condition. In medicine that’s
called a panacea, and it doesn’t exist. So this is one way
sociometry relates to action explorations.
Historically, Moreno’s vision was less rationally connected and
more intuitively woven: He started with a deep sense of the
positive potential of creativity, applying it to his immediate
relationships: Can we connect to the benefit and spiritual
uplift of all of us? He sensed further that spontaneous play
related to all this. And self-help, as he encountered the
prostitutes and other under-class people in Vienna. All this
merged with something about the deeply-felt power of
theatre—another of his interests—but for Moreno, of course, all
this would need to be re-done. So he developed improvisational
theatre a few years later. Along the way, he intuited something
about people being free to choose those with whom they
associate—i.e., the roots of sociometry.
The mind has a wonderful capacity to weave together and
rationalize shifting interests. This happens more floridly in
the enthusiasm of a truly manic person—what makes no sense to
others makes sense to him in the moment. Lower the intensity and
you have enthusiastic and bright people saying, “...and now that
I think of it, this connects, too!” It’s not always easy for
everyone to follow the thinking, but the intuitive sense of
relationship is operating for the creator. Further
rationalization lays out the logical connections; Moreno did
this a little, but not extensively; I’ve tried to connect the
dots more clearly. The point is that this moderate diversity of
interests do have an internal logic, as I see it: They are
connected by the question, “What can increase the operation of
spontaneity and creativity in the world, in systems, among
people?”
Appreciating the Non-Rational
Most folks’ behavior is not due to it being noble or even
reasonable. Adam Smith made this point in the 18th century and
it is true today. We act because it pleases us, and there is a
degree of pleasure in the raising of the mood of the group or
the pleasure given to one who we care about. This is empathy in
action. We may later find reasons to support our
attitudes—that’s called rationalization—but we act intuitively,
not because it’s calculated to please us. Of course, some
behaviors are indeed calculated, willed, planned, but these
constitute a small minority of most types of behavior.
Appendix A: Some Common Themes:
1. Favoritism. Of course there is often favoritism felt if
not shown. What is that about? How does it feel to believe that
mom liked you best?
Also sibs prefer this one
over that often for reasons not so easily explained as family
dynamics.
2. And when you go to a conference, you feel good vibes with 10%
and weird or creepy or bad vibes with 10% and a bell-shaped
curve of preference with the rest---at least that's what most
folks say, if asked. What's with that?
3. I'm checking out my hunch that no other psychology or
sociology touches this very sensitive topic.
More, I suggest that its sensitivity is a core dynamic that
doesn't get asked much about (since it isn't part of a theory),
and if you don't ask, clients tend not to bring it up---even
less so in superficial problem-solving therapy.
Appendix B: Questionnaire about Social-depth Psychology
Here are some questions that may serve to sensitize you to
the kinds of themes involved in social-depth psychology:
In your infancy or childhood, who loved you the most? In what
ways were different people loving in your childhood? Did this
sense of being loved change?
Why did people like you, do you think? How did they let you
know? Some folks know they were loved but didn’t feel
particularly liked.
Did you have anyone in your family who resonated well with
something you liked to do?
Who and in which roles?
As you entered school, do you remember whether you felt that you
were popular or unpopular? In what
ways?
In later elementary school, in what ways did you feel accepted
or rejected. Were there “in” groups and “outsiders” and which
were you in what ways?
Did you have a best friend? Do you remember his/her name? What
was that like?
Have you had any major griefs regarding friendships? Moving
away? Betrayals? Fights? Were you able to make up?
Were you ever bullied? Looking back, did you ever bully, and
what happened with that?
Were you chosen first or last for any type of team or club
activity? How did you feel about it?
In your early teen years, social-depth psychology begins to
include relationships with the opposite sex not as
peer-playmates but as objects of romance. What were your crushes
or early experiences with romance like? With sexuality?
You may have endured a break-up. How did you react?
You may have broken off with a potential romance, or from a
friendship. Why?
In junior high (middle) or high school were you part of a club,
did you hang out with some friends? What did it require to get
into your circle?
How many groups were you a part of? Joining or dropping out or
being excluded—all often have memorable stories—or not. What was
involved?
At this point in your life, identify three roles that you share
with friends. Any clubs, organizations? List them.
What groups would you like to join or become more active in?
Are there some groups that you imagine lessening your
involvement with? Why?
With these questions as warm-ups, draw a social network diagram.
Indicate what you imagine they feel towards you as well as your
feelings toward the various figures, like, neutral, dislike.
Key questions:
Were you popular or unpopular as a child? What was that like?
How about in Junior High School? High school? Were you part of a
club, did you hang out with some friends? What did it require to
get into your circle?
Were you ever bullied? Looking back, did you ever bully, and
what happened with that?
Were you chosen first or last for any type of team or club
activity? How did you feel about it?
Whom did you prefer in your nuclear family of origin? Dad, Mom,
which sibling. Whom did you think they preferred? What
about your extended family of aunts, uncles, grandparents,
cousins?
Notice that for some it’s uncomfortable to even address these
questions openly.
Appendix C: Some Common Reasons for Preferences:
* reciprocity is especially significant
* shared goals, tasks, interests, types of work or
recreation
* attractiveness--physical, social, playful, emotional,
spiritual, intellectual, artistic
* role complementarity--leader/follower, active/passive,
dominant/submissive
* role symmetry (similar qualities)--temperament,
ability,
vitality
* similar background, values, life style, education
* differences which seem intriguing or refreshing
* familiarity based on consistency or duration of
association
* nearness in place or time
* transference--similarities to other people in one's
past
* prejudice--generalizations based on cultural
conditioning
Appendix D: Networking Interests:
1. Professional:
(add other items)
➘
a: Approaches:
sociodrama drama therapy
role playing sociometry
b: Applications:
hospital
school
community
politics
recreation
business
c: Aspects:
spiritual
ethics
training
theory
2. Leisure:
(add other items)
➘
sightseeing
book stores
dining out
romance
singing
shopping
dancing
conversation
- -
- - -
- - -
- - -
-
Appendix F: Workshop Instructions
* During exercises, please stop talking when you hear the bell.
* In a choosing exercise, when you have found your partner(s),
move to the side of the room so that others can more easily find
each other.
DEFINITIONS:
Tele = interpersonal preference + some degree of reciprocity–
i.e., “rapport”
a. may have components which are due to transference
b. other components are due to reality-based factors
c. affects and is affected by group cohesion, empathy
Tele is role dependent: You might connect well with a person in
one facet of your relationship, but feel indifferent or even
somewhat negative in another facet.
Sociotelic = common interest
Psychetelic = more personal, intuitive
Please respond to adam@blatner.com